Reflections from analysis of the 2019 KS2 reading SATs: part 1

Published
01 September 2019

image in sky

 

​Analysis of the papers can take many angles and I don’t hope to cover them all in this series of blogs; instead, I intend to use analysis of the reading SATs to allow for insights into our current teaching practices in the hope of further developing and sharpening classroom pedagogy.

If I miss anything of particular interest, please do get in touch, and I will try to explore that avenue.

No change

The pass mark of 28 remained the same as in 2018. In real terms, this means that children need only have had a good stab at texts 1 and 2 to gain the marks necessary to meet the Expected Standard (EXS). Full marks on the questions relating to text 1, plus just over half marks on questions relating to text 2 would have reached the magic number.

With this in mind, securing high scores on these two texts becomes paramount for those pupils who you know are quite capable of reaching the EXS, if not quite ready to achieve GDS. It might be argued that encouraging these readers to focus more time and attention on the earlier texts – making sure that they give themselves enough time to read them well and answer questions with care – is preferable to challenging them to rush through the paper in order to get to the end. Lost marks due to rushed reading and careless mistakes on texts 1 and 2 may be a challenge to recoup through hard-won marks gained later on in the paper.

Does word count matter?

To answer this question, it is worth exploring how this year’s test compares with past papers.

According to Tim Roach’s (Twitter handle @MrTRoach) analysis of word count, we are right to feel that this year’s test was a weighty beast.

Graph 1

As his word count analysis shows, the 2019 test comprised of the most words ever presented in a KS2 reading test paper.

Can this increase in word count account for the drop in attainment at EXS at national level from 2018 to 2019 (a decrease of 3 percentage points)? And, does this national dip signify a real decline in reading standards?

In response to my first question: I would argue ‘yes’.

In response to my second question: I would argue ‘no’.

If we stick with the notion that pupils who we expect to reach the EXS need only make good ground on texts 1 & 2 in order to meet the necessary pass mark, in 2018, children had to do substantially less reading than in 2019.

To help exemplify this point, I have taken the liberty of amending Tim’s analysis grid, adding in the total word count for texts 1 & 2 only.

Graph

Now we see, with burning clarity, the reading demand made by the length of texts 1 & 2 in 2019 compared to 2018. In 2018, children had to read far fewer words (491 fewer to be precise) in order to be in with a chance of gaining enough marks to meet the EXS, than in 2019. In reality, this means that if you consider an average fluent reader – and we assume that they read a text at 150WPM (obviously this is a contentious figure and can fluctuate depending on task, context and child) – then, in 2018, a child would have had the luxury of over 3 minutes longer to spend on answering questions. This may not seem all that significant, however, any teacher will tell you that when it comes to the reading test, every second counts. Moreover, when we factor into the equation the visual impact that fewer words would have had on the confidence and enthusiasm of a reluctant or less test-savvy child, then we begin to see that the 2018 paper may have presented a more accessible challenge to most pupils. This in itself may go some way towards explaining the 3% point drop in national attainment at EXS from 2018 to 2019 – essentially, we are beginning to see that the children had to work much harder in 2019 to reach the EXS than in 2018.

So, if looking at data for EXS for the 2018 and 2019 tests doesn’t offer a fair comparison, where can we look to see if the children are getting better, or worse, at reading?

To support with this, we can turn our attention to the word count for the first two texts in the 2017 reading paper. We note from the grid above that in 2017, pupils aiming for EXS would have had to read a similar amount of words as in the 2019 test (with a difference of only 56 words). If we attribute challenge to word count alone (I am sure we wouldn’t but in fact, my analysis shows that the texts 1&2 from 2017 and 2019 were comparable in many other ways beyond word count – this will become apparent across this series of blogs), then we would anticipate a similar level of attainment at EXS for both these cohorts. Indeed, the national level data shows that 72% of pupils met the EXS in 2017 compared to 73% in 2019 – showing a small but notable 1% increase during this time.

We could therefore be bold and argue that, according to this data, children have actually improved their reading ability during this time period. If we take into account the fact that the pass mark has increased by 2 marks during this period, we may feel even bolder in our claim.

Pass mark madness!

Bearing in mind that the 2017 and 2019 tests presented a very similar level of challenge, you would expect the pass marks to have been the same. Unfortunately, they weren’t.

Imagine if the pass mark for 2019 had remained as it was in 2017 e.g. 26 rather than 28. If this had been the case, I believe we would be celebrating a rather healthy rise in standards, rather than lamenting a dip from 2018. With this in mind, a would urge schools to reflect on their data in the following  ways:

  • Calculate the data for pupils reaching the EXS in 2019 if the pass mark had been set at 26 marks.
  • Compare this figure with attainment at the EXS from 2017.

If this comparison looks favourable, I would argue that whatever you are doing to improve reading is working.

To clarify, I would argue that the 2017 test is more comparable to the 2019 test than it is to the 2018 test: in 2018, children had to read considerably less in order to have covered the information needed to answer the questions relating to texts 1 and 2, thus giving them a better chance of reaching the EXS. With this in mind, it might be fairer to look at the comparable data from 2017 to 2019 when making judgements about reading decline or improvement. When comparing these two tests, we note that there was an increase in national reading attainment at the EXS by 1%, despite children having to reach a higher pass mark. This means that despite the challenge of the test remaining high, and despite a higher pass mark, more pupils than ever met the grade this year.

Let me therefore be amongst the first to say Bravo to all the teachers out there. Let’s be sure that we are all armed with this information when we are told repeatedly – as no doubt we will be – that reading standards are declining.

I hope that this blog provides enough to get teachers thinking about some of the ways in which they might interpret the findings of this year’s reading SATs paper outcomes, and how their findings might be translated into effective classroom pedagogy.

Share this

Flexing fluency muscles with great texts (Oh…and they are free too!)

Published
20 April 2017

In this short blog, Penny Slater points to some texts that may prove useful in the last few weeks leading up to this year’s SATS.

Let’s cut to the chase…It’s early summer term. You are a Y6 teacher. You have a couple of weeks left before the 2017 Reading SATs paper. What you are probably looking for are some great texts that will give your pupils a final push to prepare for the challenge ahead? Oh…and you probably need those texts to be free and easily accessible. If so, read on….

You may be aware of BBC2’s writing competition ‘500 Words’ as a great way of inspiring young writers, but have you ever considered it as a source of material to inspire young readers? Having been running now for several years, there are hundreds of fabulous short, engaging, brilliantly written texts just waiting to be read (or in this case, plundered for suitable material to prepare children for the KS2 reading test).

I worked with the fabulous Subject Leader and Y6 teacher, Nicola Potter – reading SL at Little Green Junior School, Croxley Green –  and together we searched through the best of the best of the texts available on the site. We finally hit upon 5 texts that we felt had scope to both interest the Y6 children, and provide them with a reading challenge that was comparable in nature to the KS2 reading paper that they are just about to sit.

In order to judge comparability with the texts used in the KS2 reading paper, we ran each text through the Lexile Analysing. We have provided the Lexile rating along with the title.

Cold as Ice by Annabel Burdess

670L: comparable to How to Hide a Lion (KS1 DFE exemplification ARE)

2052 by Ophelia Spracklen

760L: comparable to Space Tourism (KS2 sample reading paper – lowest challenge)

A Stellar Job by Elizabeth Quigley

910L: comparable to The Last Leopard (KS2 reading paper 2016 – middle challenge)

Crime Doesn’t Pay…but…by Tom Foreman

900L: comparable to The Last Leopard (KS2 reading paper 2016 – middle challenge)

Fake Book by Anna Harries

1030L: comparable with The Boy in the Striped Pyjamas (KS2 DFE Exemplification ARE)

The Y6 teacher now intends to use these texts alongside the Question Stems alternating between the teacher asking some of these questions, and the children creating some of their own based on the texts read.

Either way, the hope is that these texts will provide a little last minute practice for your children, alongside some inspirational reading material (and all without costing you your time…or sanity)!

Finally, from all of us at HFL English team to all of you out there who will be supporting the Y6 children over the next few weeks to shine, we wish you the best of luck.

Share this

Do you sound good to listen to? (or ‘fluency: reading’s best-kept secret weapon’)

Published
05 September 2016

Fluency is undergoing somewhat of a revival in England. It has long been the poor relation, the magnolia paint let’s say, of reading; a general stage the ‘typical’ reader will attain when s/he reaches about a quarter past seven years old. Prior to this, children are slow readers, they are laborious decoders; they are – after all – learning how to read. Or are they…?

Don’t we already focus on fluency? Surely we aim for children to become fluent by, say, end of Year 2..? Well, one perception has been that children ‘become fluent’ after they have learnt how to read. In other words that whilst they are learning the myriad of GPCs, alternative spellings, and sufficient HFW they are bound to be slower readers. In fact children can, and indeed should, be fluently using their skills at any stage of reading. Think of fluency as a continuum, a sliding scale – rather like the old-school Hi-fi control sliders if you will. When a toddler is starting to recognise the golden arches of MacDonalds, at first the logo may need pointing out, then they may be about to shout out what it is just as the adult says, then they may get in there first, until finally they call out spontaneously ‘MacDonalds!’ In this sense, a really fluent recogniser would see an ‘m’ letter shape and call out MacDonalds, regardless of context. When a nursery-age child thinks they’ve recognised their name on a coat peg, when in fact it’s another child’s name, who just happens to share the same initial letter, they are at least fluently recognising a letter shape. This is known as the ‘logographic’ stage of reading (Frith 1985), and is a crucial step on the road to understanding and using phonics in the typical sense.

diagram

It’s like the NOFAN assessment principle (Never, Occasionally, Frequently, Always, Naturally), when we look for not just ‘frequently’, or ‘always’ using – say – full stops, but naturally doing so, to the extent it is effortlessly done and without need for proofreading. The brain is then freed-up to think of higher-order things, such as the content in writing or reading, for example, what I think the character is going to do next based on what has happened so far, things I know about him and clues the author has laid out about setting and time of day. I can connect, infer, and osmose into that world a little. Conversely, without fluency the brain is far too taxed to take in, retain and consider what is being read, let alone make inferences from it.

“Creativity floats on a sea of automaticity.” Pollard (2014)

But why is it so important? Isn’t fluency a desirable extra, something that’s more about entertaining an audience, performing and showing how much you understand about ’doing the voices’ than anything else? Wrong. It’s so much more. And it’s now been shown to actually help children become better decoders (Cunningham 1990; Whalley & Hanson 2006; Tunmer & Chapman 2012; Holliman, Wood & Sheehy 2012) and therefore potentially also has the power to reverse some types of literacy difficulties.

The commonly agreed key factors of fluency are:

  • accuracy
  • prosody/expression (pauses, intonation)
  • automaticity (or rate)

One of the really effective ways of increasing capacity for retaining what has been read (in other words, comprehending) is to teach children to ‘parse’ their reading. This involves them ‘scooping up’ in chunks of meaning or phrases, and becomes especially vital the longer the sentences become. Teaching children to read in phrases of two, three or more words helps them to scoop up and load in swathes of meaning, without putting so much load on the memory, and helps the brain focus on the main ‘big picture’ elements. Readers who are reading at a word-by-word level are unnecessarily challenging their working memories and comprehension and will struggle to have capacity left to infer and deduce. Or…they may be ‘stuck in the mud’ with slow decoding/sounding out because they are under the impression that this is what we want them to do ALL the time. If after all that slow brain-work they can still retain what the message was, they are likely either doing this for our benefit or to keep control in their ‘safe zone’; they could probably cope with speeding up and should be prompted/taught to do so. (This is where easy-reads come in handy.)

One rather exciting thing that fluent reading does, is to get the brain ‘firing on all cylinders’. This means that for children with a poorer working memory, attention-span etc, who may be at risk of reading difficulties, this can help to effectively pump up those weaker areas. New, faster, stronger neural networks can be laid down making retrieval, organisation etc much easier.

“Neurons that fire together, wire together”. (Donald Hebb, 1949)

This is why some intervention support focuses on ‘over-learning’ so that knowledge and skills become habituated. Opportunities to revisit previously-read texts let you feel good about your successful reading and there is also something rather neurally clever about being able to unconsciously predict (not guess…deduce!) what is coming up. The eye and voice come together in space and time; brain pathways reach out to connect up, creating information superhighways.

But some children just read slowly… don’t they? What if there were some main, identifiable, controllable differences between more fluent and less fluent readers? What if more fluent readers tended to be given books to read that were generally easy to decode and therefore regularly had practice at becoming more and more automatic and accurate, while less fluent readers were given books that were too hard?

Number one controllable factor: check the book is the right match for decoding ability and therefore accuracy (NC 2014, Ofsted 2014). A quick Running Record/Miscue Analysis carried out by the class teacher will identify not only whether the book is the right match but also (crucially) the next steps for precision teaching, foci for guided/individual reading etc. For more information on this valuable tool, see PM Benchmark Kit, and further guidance on matching texts in the HfL Guided Reading booklet.

National Curriculum 2014 is clear: if word reading is below age-related then children must be helped to catch up quickly, including using closely-matched texts. They are still taught age-related comprehension skills in whole-class situations, and on the texts they read, but the texts they read must be closely matched. This can mean taking the decodability/difficulty level down a notch or two. Don’t worry. Trust what you know about learning and you will see them ‘prune back to steam on’, as they remember what it feels like to understand, enjoy, successfully problem-solve and to regain some automaticity. For some children this simple revelation unlocks the ‘thrill’ and ‘will’ needed to improve the ‘skill’.

Number two controllable factor: emphasise, and teach, aspects of prosody…

“The book is talking to us.”

I can still remember so vividly the conversation that really brought the Simple View of Reading to a new reality for a vulnerable six-year-old reader. He had done a good job of decoding, even working well on alternative pronunciations, but it was still mud-like, waiting for the book to tell him the answers. He had grown from depending on others to help him, to now the book; he needed to know he already had every resource he needed within him to be a successful, active, reader. My targets for him: to re-read, putting it all together, pausing at punctuation and adding more lilts in his voice for the longer sentences to make sense.

“Does that make sense?” …led to a confused face.

“What would you say if it was you?”…still confusion.

“The book is talking to us. If you were the book, how would you say this?”

He got it. He knew what I meant. His eyes widened though, as if I had asked him to disobey a school rule. He had sounded out, he had told me what the words said, but that seemingly wasn’t enough for this rather persistent pedant sat next to him right now. Glancing up at the print-out of Superman with his laser vision from our early focus on steady eye-pointing, he looked at me and had another go. Better.

Okay. Time to get radical. I covered the book and had him repeat the sentence after me, phrase by phrase, rehearsing the voices, lilts and swoops. The spark grew in his eyes. With a warmed-up oral rehearsal fresh in his mind I revealed the page.

“Try that again.” I said.

Whoosh. Scooping almost in hesitant disbelief at times, he read. He went on, gobbling up the page as if it was a feast of tumbling visions and no longer words. For a moment we were there in the book in that moment with the character.

Number three controllable factor: increase opportunities for developing automaticity.

“Easy reading makes reading easy.” (Tim Rasinski)

When re-reading texts or reading easier texts, children are then freed up to comprehend, infer, internalise new vocabulary encountered etc. It has also been shown – along with letter-name knowledge – to have huge correlation with spelling accuracy. Brooks (2013) showed that a fluency and comprehension-focused intervention had a bigger impact, especially for disadvantaged children, on spelling ability at Y6-7 than one based on discrete phonics alone.

“Spelling and reading build and rely on the same mental representation of a word. Knowing the spelling of a word makes the representation of it sturdy and accessible for fluent reading.” (Catherine Snow et al, 2005)

Other ideas for developing automatic decoding and improving rate or pace:

  • Repeated readings of books or passages (could be timed)
  • Take turns reading page/sentence etc so they hear a fluent model
  • Reader’s Theatre/playscripts
  • Poetry reading
  • My turn, Your turn – get them to repeat a sentence the exact way you read it, and if it’s not the same, re-model and ask again. Get them to reflect: “Did you sound good to listen to?” “Did it sound like talking?”
  • After decoding ‘work’: “Now put that all together so it sounds smooth/good to listen to/like talking.”
  • Encouraging decoding into chunks before going down to individual phonemes, e.g. use of syllables, morphology
  • Finger frame a line: “Read to here, like talking.”; “Make it sound good to listen to.”
  • Have them use a Speech and Language Therapy ‘phone’ (try TTS) so they can hear themselves amplified. [These can also be good for those struggling to sound out when spelling, e.g. confusing vowel sounds.]
  • Try recording them and playing back for review as above
  • Practise spelling words that were hard to read, and will occur again frequently. If using a ‘Look, Say, Cover, Write, Check’ method, ensure you have them look for the tricky bits and self-check after each go.

References:

Bodman, S. & Franklin, G. (2014). Which Book and Why? Using Book Bands and boo

Join our next cohort of the Reading Fluency Project

Share this

Early findings from the KS2 reading fluency project

Published
11 October 2017

With 150 pupils across Hertfordshire now involved in HFL’s KS2 Reading Fluency Project, Penny Slater reflects on what has been learned from the project so far.

Last week, HFL officially launched the first round of the KS2 Reading Fluency Project, involving 20 Herts schools. Based on our work carried out in a number of schools throughout 2016/17, we have good reason to believe that this project will go a long way towards supporting many of their year 6 pupils, who are currently at risk of falling behind, to reach the Expected Standard in the 2018 reading test.

Armed now with a great amount of qualitative data, and a growing database of quantitative data, we are in a position to share some of our early findings from this work.

The project began on a very small scale, in one school, where the SL was keen to ensure that those pupils who entered KS2 at a 2b/2c reached the EXS by the end of year 6. At the time when I was working with the school, they were concerned that many of these pupils, who were then in year 5 and year 6, would not do so. Observations of a sample of these pupils reading a well-pitched ARE text, indicated that they were far from fluent: their reading sounded choppy; robotic and monotonous. In addition, they had a disregard for punctuation, and they lacked the ability to monitor comprehension as they read (indicated by the fact that they often mis-read words, or at times completely missed out words – or whole lines of text – without realising and self-correcting). Their reading comprehension was poor (as judged by their inability to attempt many of the oral comprehension questions asked after reading the text). The school sought a swift and effective teaching strategy that would – as the lowest indicator of success  – support these children to quickly gain ground in advance of the impending test, but would – at best – turn these switched-off readers onto the joys of this particular pastime.

We decided upon a strategy that we initially named ‘modelled fluent reading’ sessions, but now, as the project launches into full swing, this has evolved into a more sophisticated model. The current project supports teachers to embed the following strategies into regular reading sessions of engaging, well-pitched ARE texts: modelled fluent reading; text marking; echo reading; opportunities for repeated re-readings and performance. The schools are asked to work with six children over an 8-week period, offering a double-dose of guided reading: session 1 to focus on modelled fluent reading practice and echo reading, and session 2 to focus on comprehension development. Following trials over the summer term in a number of different schools, led by HfL advisors Sabrina Wright and Kathy Roe, we can now present our early findings:

It works (for most children):

Of the 29 children who took part in the summer trials, 23 children made gains in comprehension of between 4 months and 5 years. Out of the number of pupils who made 4 months-plus progress in reading comprehension over the 8-week period, 16 children made over one year’s progress (10 pupils in that group made over 2 years’ progress!).

Data was gained using the YARC reading comprehension test.

It did not work for a small number of children:

Our trials helped us to refine our understanding of whom this project really helped. Six children did not make more than 8 weeks progress in their comprehension following the project. Much of our discussion following these trials have focused on what it was about these particular children that meant that they did not benefit from the project. As is often the case, each child prompted a different theory, but factors we have considered are as follows:

  • Dislike of the project method – one child was particularly shy and disliked the reading aloud element of the project
  • Pupil selection – in some cases, pupils’ fluency didn’t seem to be the barrier to learning and those pupils therefore didn’t make as much progress with their comprehension. We refined pupil selection criteria as a result

All of these considerations have enabled us to better support schools in selecting pupils who are most likely to make gains as a result of the methods used in the project.

More than simply reading aloud:

Repeatedly, we discovered that simply reading aloud to the children (despite doing so in a perfectly fluent and engaging fashion) did not support their comprehension development as much as we had anticipated. Because we found this aspect of our study so interesting, we explored it repeatedly during our trials. Through doing so, we reaffirmed our observation that echo reading (where a child has the opportunity to hear the words on the page spoken by their own voice following modelling by an ‘expert’ reader) allowed for better comprehension, compared to when the text was simply read aloud to the children. We have not yet gained quantitative data to support this finding (partly because the YARC test does not allow for this analysis) but we witnessed it time and time again during our observations and trial sessions.

Watch your speed:

Most teachers lament the fact that many of their children simply do not read quickly enough to get through the reading paper in time to have a hope of reaching the expected standard. I have long wondered whether this is the case, or whether the problem is that they read it too quickly, and too passively, merely hoping that by passing their eyes over the words, the meaning hidden within them will leap into their panicked brains. If they do this, then they will probably end up having to read the text over and over again during the test simply because it did not go in the first time. It might be more time-efficient, to read it a bit slower, but better.

Our small-scale studies showed that out of the 23 children who made gains in comprehension, seven children actually reduced their reading rate – their reading got slower! Eight of the pupils increased their reading rate (although all marginally) and the remaining eight recorded the same reading rate as their pre-intervention score.

Our study also revealed that prior to the project, the selected pupils rarely self-corrected as they read, demonstrating a lack of understanding. One child replaced the word monk with monkey and continued on, unaware. The same child read again from a ‘cold’ piece at the end of the 8 week project and re-read sentences and words for sense as she went along, demonstrating that she was ‘taking in’ the text. This may have slowed her pace, but aided her comprehension and retrieval.

It is important to get the simple things right:

When summing up our work to teachers and other colleagues, we have been struck by how the techniques we propose could be neatly summed up in a few minutes – or a few sentences, as above. However, as we state on our whole day project launch, it takes time, effort and skill to get the simple things right. In order to effectively model fluent reading, teachers have to be acutely aware of what fluency is, and what it sounds like when reading an age-related text. Prosody being perhaps the most challenging aspect of fluency, we support teachers to apply their own prosodic knowledge to the analysis of a challenging text that would test even an expert reader’s prowess. This proves to be a real eye-opener for many teachers and helps them realise what a door-opener prosodic knowledge is to reading comprehension. Teachers also need to gain confidence in knowing what a good text choice looks like if it is both going to inspire a reluctant reader to read, and prepare them for success in the KS2 test. Finally, teachers need the time and space to reflect on their current practice and consider how this is contributing – or not – to the development of prosodic understanding. All of these things take time, practice and most importantly, headspace: something that we try to offer on our launch day.

Children enjoy it!

We knew from our early work – which relied predominantly on watching the transformation in children’s reading following a well-planned modelled fluency session – that children got so much more out of the sessions than simply better comprehension skills. When we observed children complete a cold read of an unknown text after 20 minutes or so of intensive reading work, the children had changed. Not only did they read better and certainly they understood a lot more, but there was more to be observed: they sat up a little straighter; they turned the pages with a little more gusto; they inhaled breath a little less dramatically between each sentence (in the baseline observation, one child gulped air between each sentence as if he were preparing for the next bout of a wrestling match). Most notably, fatigue did not hit quite so hard and so fast. The children kept going, or ‘performed’ (to mimic the language we use on the project), for longer, and appeared to enjoy doing so. The results of the pupil voice surveys from our summer trials confirmed that the techniques used in the project go further than simply preparing children for a reading test. Below are some of the quotes from children and teachers who took part in the summer trials.

Has it made you feel differently about reading?

– I didn’t think I noticed it helped, but I’m reading a lot more.

– I used to read David Walliams, but now I’m reading Alex Ryder because I liked the suspense stories we read.

– I’m reading more mysteries and suspense now.

– It’s made me step up the hours.  I’m reading more now.

– I’m reading with more expression.  It made me enjoy reading more and choose a different variety of books.

Teacher Comments:

-The children have really enjoyed the sessions and having a challenging text.

– The children seem a lot more confident.

– The children in this group seemed more resilient during an end of year assessment.  They are the children that may have previously given up.

– This project enabled me to know the Y6 text pitch really well.

– It meant they had exposure to texts like the end of year assessments.

– I have struggled to get ‘X’ to pick up a single narrative this year, and now he is reading Roald Dahl.

– The children have been taking the skills they’ve been learning and applying these outside of the sessions.

We acknowledge that we are in the early stages of our work, but with 20 schools (and 150+ pupils) launching whole-heartedly into the project last week, we look forward to developing our knowledge of how these techniques can help to raise reading standards, and to sharing these findings in the near future.

Read more about the HFL Reading Fluency Project or contact reading.fluency@hfleducation.org

With thanks to the following schools for their participation in the summer trials:

Beechfield Primary School, Watford

Woodhall Primary School, Watford

Summerswood Primary School, Borehamwood

De Havilland Primary School, Hatfield

With special thanks to Reedings Junior School, Sawbridgeworth for their early involvement in the project.

Reading that informed the project:

  • Dowhower (1994) Repeated Reading Revisited: Research Into Practice, Reading and Writing Quarterly, 10
  • EEF (2017) Improving Literacy in Key Stage Two, Guidance Report
  • Heitin (2015) Literacy Expert: Weak Readers Lack Fluency, Not Critical Thinking
  • Heitin (2015) Reading Fluency Viewed as Neglected Skill, Education Week, http://www.edweek.org
  • Marcell Putting Fluency on a Fitness Plan, The Reading Teacher Vol 65 issue 4
  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (2000) Report of the National Reading Panel; Teaching Children to Read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction, US government Printing Office
  • Rasinski, Yildirim, & Nageldinger (2011) Building Fluency Through the Phrased Text Lesson, The Reading Teacher Vol 65 issue 4
  • Rasinski (2012) Why Reading Fluency Should be Hot!, The Reading Teacher Vol 65 Issue 8
  • Rasinski (2014) Delivering Supportive Fluency Instruction, Reading Today
  • Rasinski & Nageldinger (2016) The Fluency Factor: Authentic Instruction and Assessment for Reading Success in the Common Core Classroom, Teachers College Press
Share this

Read like nobody is watching

Published
04 February 2020

figures sitting on a newspaper

 

Thoughts occur when you are inspired. On this occasion, my inspiration came from a session that I attended, delivered by my colleague, Martin Galway, entitled 'What do we mean when we talk about reading (and writing) fluency?' This session was delivered at Oxford Reading Spree in October 2019, and these are the thoughts that it provoked.

During the session, Martin referred to the difference between ‘in the moment’ or automatic processing in reading, and processing that takes place after the event of actually ‘reading’ the text. I am paraphrasing and extrapolating from Martin’s exact words, so you may wish to refer to Martin’s original blog linked to the session for where his talk went from here.

Sometimes, depending on how the reading act has been framed,  children read for a less-than-desirable purpose – usually because they have been told to read by a teacher and therefore they get on with it, without perhaps bringing much more than an attitude of ‘let’s get this business over and done with’ to the table. These children could also be classified as passive readers. These are the children, who when faced with the most hilarious/terrifying/interesting text, barely raise an eyebrow. They have figured out the real way to read (or at least they think they have) based upon the reading experiences that have been presented to them on numerous occasions. Having learnt the deal, they tackle a reading task as follows: they pseudo-read the text (the eyes may be on the page but the mind is probably somewhere else) before waiting for the inevitable questions that will follow the piece – these can be oral but are often written. At this point, they reluctantly drag their gaze back through the entire piece hunting for words or phrases that might match the question brief. To the passive reader, the text represents little more than a blob on the page. A first read may not begin to involve such processes as sematic inferencing, visualising, summarising, prediction etc, and as such the reader may lack a sure, or even partial, grasp of the text.  If asked to share something of their understanding, some resort to re-reading the whole text again, from top to bottom whilst others may well pluck information from the text in the hope that it hits the mark.

It is easy to see how this passive form of reading could become habitual for the child who has repeatedly encountered the ‘reading followed by questions’ formula. It is also easy to see why teachers might go down this route. The ‘text then question’ formula does after all mimic the SATs reading test that children will face at the end of KS1 and 2. But, as reflective practitioners, we must become persistent in asking ourselves difficult questions; in this case, just because reading is tested in this way, does it mean that it should be taught in this way?

To clarify, I am aware that very few teachers would simply present children with a cold text, expect them to read it, and then present them with questions relating to the piece to answer, without adequate discussion. However, I would argue that even when post-reading discussion based on key questions takes place, we may still be reinforcing the notion that children can get away with a passive first read. When following this approach, the teacher is directly modelling a distorted reading behaviour; that is, reinforcing to the children that good readers read a text and then they think about it. I do believe that post-reading questioning is important, however, if we want to shunt passive readers towards becoming active readers, then we need to place greater emphasis on modelling the ‘in the moment’ reading behaviours that accompany an engaged reading experience, rather than focusing exclusively on modelling the post-reading behaviours that allow us to reflect back upon a text at a deeper level.

If we want to create active readers, then we need to remind ourselves of what they look like. Active readers can be a joy to behold, especially in child-form. Hopefully you will have all seen one in action. When reading silently, their heads are abuzz with sound. Internally, they are engaged in a meaningful expressive read of the text at hand. When they get to a funny bit, they laugh. When they get to a scary bit, they shiver. When they are perplexed, the eyebrow raises. When the teacher arrives at a cliff-hanger in the class read-aloud, they provide the obligatory doof…doof…doof (Eastender style). In adult form, they can be even more amusing – although it can be unsettling when you sit next to one on a train! Ultimately, we want all of our children to become active readers. Our mission should be to create whole carriages of train chortlers!

Hopefully we are agreed that this is what we want to achieve, but how?

Below you will find a list of my top tips aimed at moving passive readers, to active ‘in the moment’ readers.

Prepare them for reading

If you know that there is a funny/scary/interesting/disgusting bit in the text that you are about to read with the class, prepare them for it. For example,

‘When I first read this text, children, I burst out laughing at one bit. I wonder if you will do the same?’

Following the read, praise the children who reacted so they know that this is the right and proper way to engage with a text.

Re-brand silent reading

I dislike the term ‘silent reading’. I worry that it may inadvertently imply to a child that silence occurs both outside and inside the brain whilst reading. Instead, I prefer to use the term ‘busy brain reading’. When this term is established, you can start talking to the children about the busy brain activities that may be taking place inside their heads at the point of reading.

Modelling busy brain reading

It is not enough to model post-reading thinking. If we want to nurture active readers, we must nurture ‘in the moment’ reading. This could be as simple as reading the text to the children, and then returning to the beginning and re-reading, but this time pausing to share reading reactions as they occur. There will be far too many links/reactions/thoughts/strategies taking place in your head at any one point to warrant modelling them all. The challenge of teaching is knowing what your children can do, and what they cannot yet do. It is the latter that will inform the aspects of busy brain reading that you choose to model.

Prompt and provoke

Instead of simply listening to children reading, why not take the opportunity to listen to children think whilst reading. This is easily done. When working with a child one on one, or in a small group, invite them to read, but let them know that you will be interested in their thoughts as they read. Invite them to pause when they come across anything of interest/concern/alarm etc and tell you about it. In my experience, this really is a game-changing. Instantly, the children know that you expect more from them that simply barking at the text: instead, you expect them to both read and think at the same time – this is the goal of the active reader.

With these simple approaches, we may go some way towards nurturing engaged readers; children who read like nobody is watching!

If you are interested in considering further strategies for promoting active reading in the primary classroom, please join us at one of our KS2 Reading Fluency Project Roadshows, which will be taking place in various locations around this country this year.

Please visit the HFL Reading Fluency Project page for full details of forthcoming events.

If you are keen to host one of our events, please do get in touch at reading.fluency@hfleducation.org

Share this

Fluency: the bridge from phonics to comprehension

Published
15 December 2020
‘Words mean more than what is set down on paper. It takes the human voice to infuse them with shades of deeper meaning.’

Maya Angelou – I know why the caged bird sings

Highlights, key learning and juicy tips from the session – written by Penny Slater

  • Begin each reading session with a fluency exercise (aka a song!). Why? It’s reading! When children sing a song with the words in front of them, they are accessing great material to develop their fluency. Also, children love to sing (as do most adults)!
  • Make good use of poetry. Many poems are short and accessible and can do wonders to boost the confidence of weaker readers.
  • Fluency instruction sits between word study and comprehension, hence the reference to it being a bridge. Some children can traverse that bridge by themselves, whilst others need a helping hand to get across. To begin the journey, children need a good foundation with words. Word Ladders are a great way for children to get playful with word-based learning, whilst at the same time bolstering their vocabulary knowledge. Resource: Daily Word Ladders
  • The goal of phonics instruction is to get readers to the point where they do not (consciously) need to use phonics! Aim for ‘automaticity’ – reading words with no apparent mental effort so that we are releasing mental resources for the task of reading comprehension.
  • Phrasing matters - children who read words within sentences word by word will have a hard time extracting meaning. Pay particular attention to children who voice generic words e.g. prepositions, or perhaps determiners (for example, a child who reads the word ‘the’ in isolation from the words around it when there is no good reason for that emphasis).

Instructional tools for teaching fluency:

  • Model fluent reading: in order to become fluent, you have to know what fluency sounds like. The answer lies in reading to our children.
    • Top tip: When discussing a text with the children, be sure not to just talk about the story, but how the story was read to them!
  • Assisted reading: reading alongside the children in unison so that they can hear your voice and follow your lead.
  • Practice:
    • Type 1: Wide reading practice – encouraging lots of reading of a range of different genres and text types. As opposed to little and often; aim for lots and often!
    • Type 2: Deep reading practice – repeated reading of the same text until adequate fluency is achieved.
    • Top tip: To encourage repeated re-reads, invite children to perform the text they have been practising to as many ‘Lucky Listeners’ as possible. Each Lucky Listener can sign the back of the text. PS. Dogs make great listeners (Cats? Not so much!)
    • Further reading: Repeated Read Alouds May Lead to Reading Success for Young Children
  • Focus on phrasing: Teach high frequency words within phrases, rather than as individual words (see point above about the word ‘the’). Use text marking to guide a phrased reading. Aim for the children to ultimately ‘perform’ the text without the text marking to guide them – by this point, the phrasing should be internalised.

Tim ended the session with a poem:

The Definition of Success by R W Emerson

To laugh often and much;

 to win the respect of intelligent people

and the affection of children;

 to earn the appreciation of honest critics

and to endure the betrayal of false friends.

To appreciate beauty;

to find the best in others;

 to leave the world a bit better

whether by a healthy child, a garden patch, or a

 redeemed social condition;

to know that even one life

has breathed easier

because you have lived.

This is to have succeeded.

Thank you for this inspirational session, Tim. You certainly earnt our respect, as well as our gratitude.

Ways to get in touch with Tim:

www.timrasinski.com

On Tim’s blog site, you will find a blog written by Penny Slater and Kathy Roe about the HFL Education Reading Fluency Project: strategies and outcomes.

Twitter @timrasinski1

Tim posts three lessons a week on Twitter

Tim is author of award-winning text: Mega-Book of Fluency

Share this

What do we mean when we talk about reading (and writing) fluency?

Published
02 October 2019

  

image of text

September: return to work; note the chill in the air; resist the urge to swap too many anxiety dream stories; present at ResearchEd. Those last two may be related .

Last year I had the privilege of presenting at the ResearchEd national conference with Megan Dixon (@DamsonEd) and the sadness of not presenting with Sinéad Gaffney (@shinpad1), our absent co-author. There we considered the dominant approaches to reading instruction in primary schools, weighed them up against each other, and found that they are all quite handy actually. We are rather keen on this outcome. It’s comfortingly old fashioned for teachers to have a range of teaching approaches to draw upon as they refine their instincts and make the daily choices that only they can make. The year before that I had spoken about reading and grammar and the links cover those sessions sufficiently well to be getting on with.

This year, my session was titled 'What do we mean when we talk about reading (and writing) fluency?' The session took as its focus the following key strands:

  • defining reading and exploring some conceptual frameworks that begin to account for the underlying/constituent processes that come together in proficient reading and on the journey towards it;
  • a brief and tentative exploration of some of the potential causal factors in dysfluent reading;
  • a brief, far less tentative exploration of how policy and online discussion of reading can sometimes be less helpful than intended;
  • a recap and update on what we mean by reading fluency, why subtle distinctions in how this is defined can have serious consequences, and some suggested approaches and reading to support the development of fluent reading;
  • a discussion of some recent work exploring how to make the links between reading and writing more seamless for our students, so that work on reading fluency more efficiently informs work on writing development – composition and authorial intent in particular.

There are too many slides to share here – it really was a packed session – so I have cherry picked those that are either central, or that seemed to draw a lot of interest.  For the sake of space-saving, I have grouped slides and then provide a short commentary below them.  Please feel free to get in touch if you would like further details or a PDF with a more comprehensive set of slide images.

 

text

 

The session began with introductions and a credit to key collaborators and fellow reading warriors Megan  and Sinéad who have done much of the thinking that informs so much of this work.  It’s important to credit, especially in reading where messages sometimes seem to be bent out of shape almost as quickly as efficient word reading.  

 

text example

 

As in all the sessions that I share with Megan and Sinead, reading was defined to some degree here, as that seemingly simple term (“it’s just reading, isn’t it?”) can mean different things, in different contexts, and according to different perspectives.  Note the shared thread of complexity: Stuart and Stainthorp take care from the outset of their book to make clear that whilst key components of reading are necessarily outlined and discussed in isolation, reading is an integrative act.  Think of Scarborough’s reading rope and the way that it cleverly illustrates the increasingly interwoven threads of reading as the developing reader moves towards proficiency.

 Incidentally, Stuart and Stainthorp’s book is an extremely helpful title, which really should be receiving more recommendations, relative to some of the books enjoying greater currency on Twitter and other platforms, particularly given its attention to the whole journey of learning to read.  Also recommended is the paper cited in the right-hand slide, a well-structured and comprehensive overview of much of the most up-to-date thinking around reading.

 

text example

 

Given its influence in the literature, and in the way that it underpins the curriculum, there had to be a discussion of the Simple View of Reading (SVoR). First, we explored how the SVoR works as a conceptual framework.  We discussed how there are two components at play – language comprehension processes and word recognition skills – and that both have to be at play for reading to happen.  It is a multiplicative model.  If one aspect is missing, the effect of multiplying by 0 holds true here.  The act of reading, as defined for the purposes of this session, has to include both the act of decoding words (or "lifting them off the page” as often seems to be said) and understanding what the words, phrases, clauses, sentences (or lines and verses, say) come together to mean.  I have tried to word that carefully, but I am equally mindful that this is a blog and not an essay.  A significant part of my 2017 session was spent on exploring how much mental work is going on in order to read the words, integrate meanings across word and phrases, and then to make similar links across sequences of sentences, across sections or paragraphs, and ultimately across texts.  It should never be forgotten quite how staggering  this routine achievement of reading actually is in terms of human evolution.  Maryanne Wolf’s Proust and the Squid remains possibly the most beautifully written account of reading, in these terms.

 

text example

 

To further dig deeper into reading, Kate Cain’s very helpful diagram was used.  Megan, Sinead, and I particularly like to use this model as it is non-threatening (some diagrammatic representations of reading  are not so digestible on first sight) and more particularly, because it works with the simple view of reading . Note the top floor of the house. However, this representation is carefully constructed to work up from the ground floor with the implicit understanding that each successive storey rests on those lower down.  No ground floor?  You won’t get a good night’s sleep in the upstairs bedrooms.

I’ve spent too long thinking about the Simple View of Reading over the past four years.  In the course of that thinking, I have been able to observe less-than-effective reading practice that, on occasion, has happened because of the simplicity of the framework.  There is a surprising degree of scope for misapprehension.  Simple is sometimes best; sometimes it is simply misunderstood. Rather than take up too much more space on this here, let me get to my central point.  We want as many of our children to achieve fluent reading as humanly possible, and as efficiently as possible.  It is increasingly agreed now  that fluency is not simply rate and/or accuracy, but also incorporates expressive, phrased reading. That is, the reader demonstrates prosodic awareness and in doing so attends to meaning as they read, adapting the way in which words (and word parts)  are stressed, grouped, pitched and more, to reflect the dynamics of spoken language and, where necessary, the voice that is called for in the sad part of a story, or in delivering an important speech.

 Or the breathlessness of a packed ResearcEd session.

If fluency rests on an appropriate pace, and accuracy – often discussed as components of word reading – as well as prosody – more obviously related to meaning - then fluency extends across both dimensions of the Simple View of Reading.  I’ve tried to represent what I mean here:

 

diagram

 

The yellow arrow indicates how fluency is often discussed but even here we have a problem.  To be fully accurate in terms of pronunciation (or phonological representation) of homographs (words with the same spelling, but different sound patterns and meanings)  we often need to draw upon the context in order to read the word accurately.  So for example: “To read this carefully will lead you to rhyme ‘read’ and ‘lead’ with need”.   That use of context or syntax forces us beautifully, and hopefully, smoothly into the realm of language comprehension, and ideally, language comprehension on-the-run so that reading is smooth, responsive and only occasionally needs a “go back and check that bit” moment. To wrap up this section, it might mean that  the fluency ‘room’ in Professor Cain’s house might need to be extended into the ‘Language Comprehension wing’ depending on how fluency is being defined. 

 

text example

 

I would just now like to share a link to a paper by Kate Nation that is happily available for free access and that goes into far richer detail about why the Simple View of Reading may benefit from a degree more complexity: www.tandfonline.com/. Let’s just say this paper has scratched some longstanding itches, and move on.

 

Image 7

 

The next section explored the research around difficulties in comprehension.  This is a complex area that I cannot hope to do any kind of justice to here.  However, I would flag that all too often, those that might be vulnerable to comprehension difficulties are not identified until texts become sufficiently complex and require integration of more elements, of increasing complexity. It worries me a great deal when I read that you 'cannot teach comprehension' on social media.  I think that is hard to argue with if you take that statement very literally.  Comprehension is the product of reading and you cannot directly teach what form(s) that takes, or doesn't take, in the reader's mind. Nor should we.  Reading is not the transfer of a single idea or interpretation from writer to text, text to reader.  These impressions, or the gist, or the mental model to be more complete, sit, unhelpfully or helpfully depending on each reading experience, in the reader's mind.  We can try to dig it out, unearth it, bring it to the surface, but our tools are not always as sharp as we would like them to be and things can be lost, distorted, or broken in the process of excavation.  There is also the issue around what we even mean when we use the term 'comprehension'.  Are we talking about what a reader comes to understand of a text, or something even bigger?  Less helpfully, are we talking about a form of reading exercise, much like SATs, in which reading is followed by a series of questions, often organised to reflect assessment domains?

If we are talking about the latter, we may not be attending to the sorts of activity that help to develop confident, actively responsive readers.  Wayne Tennent  has spoken and written (see his 2015 book) on the tensions between where research suggests we might best direct our energies, and where energies are often directed largely due to the messages inferred from accountability measures for reading.  I discussed this tension in the session: how we should look towards developing 'automatic' or 'online' inference-making (occurring in the act, across a given text) over 'controlled' or 'offline' inference-making, promoted by  exercises that occur once the reading is done, such as a SATs style practice exercise. Back to my concerns about whether or not we can 'teach comprehension'. We could discuss this over the course of endless blogs, but life is short, and blogs are meant to be too.  However, there are plenty of things that we can do to support those that might, or do, struggle with comprehension and I worry that we might sideline these needs in pursuit of yet another simplistic view of reading. Having said all of  that, we turned to the practical. One such aspect of our work that should be supportive of enhanced comprehension is to work to develop fluent reading and to provide opportunities to experience what it is to read with fluency.

In order to provide practical solutions, the slides above formed part of a longer suite of around 12  slides that were used to explore time and cost-efficient strategies for developing fluency in reading. We deliver a range of exciting Reading Fluency CPD and projects now across KS1, KS2 and KS3.  You can find out more details here. Some of the headline approaches are detailed below.

 

diagram

 

The session then moved on to explore some of my more recent work on the links between what we can learn from some explicit work on reading fluency, and how this might complement our work on writing composition.

 

Image 9

 

As you can see from the top left slide, I have adopted the Möbius strip as a symbol of the seamlessness with which we want the development of skills, knowledge, and understanding in both reading and writing to inform each other.  They are inextricably linked, reciprocal sets of processes and are both shot through with language learning. Yet some children (and adults) are quick to decouple them to varying degrees without realising that they have done so.  One of the propositions of the session was that perhaps we could take what we have learnt from our work in developing fluency (read it like you mean it)  and then take a step back further to ask our young writers to consider the needs of their intended reader (write like you mean them to read it).  Donald Grave’s once referred to “selfish” writers and asked that we work to guide the development of “selfless writers”.  Writers that keep their intended audiences firmly in mind, not just in terms of sometimes tokenistic, or even statutory talk of ‘audience’ and ‘purpose’ but in terms of envisioning how the reader will be served, or toyed with, or manipulated, or informed,  or simply kept interested.  In this, it also builds on a programme of extended CPD that my colleague Jane and I have been developing. 

Back to the ResearchEd session, the slides acknowledge the importance of developing automaticity in both spelling and handwriting, if only in terms of motivation to write. Please note these are huge factors for many, if not most, reluctant or wary writers.  The central theme I then went on to develop related to chronology and cohesion, and how some carefully sequenced work on these aspects of writing might help to develop our young writer’s own sense of what it feels like to write fluid text.

As in some of my earlier blogs, the influence of Myra Barr’s and Valerie Cork’s The Reader in the Writer (CLPE, 2001) extends into this work.  One aspect of this influence manifests in the encouragement to focus not just on a linear written journey from A to B to C, but a journey that might meander (purposefully of course) forwards, backwards, or sideways in time.  Or that perhaps lingers just a little longer here or there, providing the right kinds of detail and space for the reader to conjure up a more fully realised mental model that draws upon fluent skilled reading: making inferences, speculating, and receiving cues: this is a sinister part – please whisper; this is action – please shift gear.  As such, we have strayed into the endlessly fascinating realm of narratology. We also talked about some of the science involved in negotiating such ideas as readers, but this blog has probably more than outstayed its welcome.

I wish I could spend longer outlining what we covered in the session but I am heading towards a deadly word count.  If you have unresolved questions or points that require further clarification, please do get in touch with the Primary English team.

Share this

Ten top tips for core subject leaders

Published
27 August 2019

Kirsten Snook, English Adviser and course trainer for ‘Becoming a Highly Effective Subject Leader’, reflects on what makes the biggest differences to increasingly busy subject leaders and with increasingly tight budgets. Drawing on feedback from course delegates, she outlines some top tips that have helped them this year to really see the fruits of their labours.

As we near the end of this academic year and look ahead to the next, it’s a good time for subject leaders to stop, take stock and reflect on how the year has gone. For subject leaders, often the measure of your impact is through pupil outcomes… percentages, data, ITAFs, SATs etc. But don’t forget all the other angles to your role – the leadership skills, deep subject knowledge and getting strategic when improving teaching and learning. All these things have a massive impact on pupil outcomes too. In this bite-sized article, I summarise the key thoughts and reflections of the fantastic subject leaders I have worked with over this past year, in the hope that some practice-sharing tips help you to get in the mood for next year.

1. Don’t try to fix everything at once!

You may feel that everything needs sorting at once, but it is so much better to do one thing really well and to be able to sustain the change than to scrape the surfaces of many things and find nothing embeds. Remember what conscientious creatures we teachers are – we will never feel finished, no matter how many late nights we put in. On our subject leader training we ask colleagues to identify the one thing they want to see a change in over the year, and then weave that through everything they do, from action-planning to coaching.

Ensure you are happy that you are seeing change in a key aspect, that this is across the board, well-embedded, well-evidenced and that this will be sustainable. The changes you see will improve that area of practice but also there will be transferable benefits such as increased reflectiveness, openness, willingness to change, and aspects of pedagogy that can also be applied in other areas or subjects. You will also get people on board if they feel they are not having millions of demands made of them by the one subject leader… they will want to ‘get on your bus’ and come with you!

2. Do some ‘quick-win’ monitoring activities

Yes, a thorough trawl of pupil books or full-on lesson observations are sometimes necessary, especially at the beginning of the year or when you’re new to the role of SL, or even when doing a full ‘stocktake’ of how English currently fares in your school. But sometimes, and once in role, you know what you’re looking for or checking up on, and so you can make targeted use of your time. Ofsted prefer Learning Walks these days, which is a great ‘3-in-1’ way of getting a flavour of lessons, having a quick flick through some books and talking to the children about their learning. We would normally call these activities Lesson Observations, Work Scrutiny and Pupil Voice. How much nicer ‘Learning Walk’ sounds though! There will be times you need to share difficult messages – make this one of those times when your approach can be less judgemental and more developmental. It helps everyone feel more relaxed, respected and able to go about their usual brilliant business. Subject leaders often comment on how freeing this form of monitoring can be, and are supported throughout the course to use the proformas within our PA+ website here.

3. Don’t forget to evaluate!

Sounds obvious I know, but one of the big benefits of taking a lighter-touch approach to monitoring, as described above, is that it tends to leave you with more time to think about the ‘so what’ and the ‘what next’. You want the monitoring activity to serve its purpose of helping you to find out the strengths and improvements in the aspect you’re developing – and we can often overlook these – and the bits that could be further developed and how you know. Make sure you then leave time to think about the ‘what next’. Precisely what do you need to do next, with whom and when will you do it? That is the crucial part in terms of taking your subject forward. Would someone benefit from support with planning? Bundling in with you in your Guided Writing session?  Or even helping lead a tiny bit of a staff meeting on something you’ve seen them do well? Invest time in plotting these into calendars, annotating your action plan and you will really start to feel on top of things.

4. Walk the walk

Lead by example; if you are asking colleagues to make a change to their practice (eg adopt a new strategy) make sure you do it first. After all, it may not even work in your school – you won’t know until you trial it. The best next step is to ‘scale it up’, eg ask a friend or two to try it out for a period of time, and feed back to you warts and all. What needed tweaking to suit their style? Which tweaks affected impact and in what ways? What degree of licence/autonomy can be taken when implementing the strategy?

After this, and with a wealth of pros and cons, feedback and evaluation you’ll have considered and acted on, you are in both a strong position to roll out your change and also to know how to support colleagues with implementing it. See the Education Endowment Foundation website here for more on this process of ‘scaling up’ before ‘rolling out’.

5. Be prepared to switch between support and challenge

Yes, we want to support our colleagues (who are often our friends too), but we must remember we have a job to do. We are primarily there for the children, and if you ask any colleague who maybe feels resistant to a change why they are there, they should agree. Some things can help smooth the way for challenge though, such as the point above and things such as clear deadline dates, reminders, examples of What a Good One Looks Like (WAGOLL), and differentiating approaches to differing personalities… you know, the kinds of things we do with other, smaller-sized learners. New learning can put anyone at any age outside of their comfort zone so think about what you know works with little learners and how you can apply this to older ones. The key differences being emotional baggage that older learners may well have (“I can never get this right!”) and seeing through patronising talk (“Oh I see you’re just trying to make me feel valued”). Get genuine. Get personal. Level with them a little. Then gently let them know that everyone does need to do X, and why, and that you are there for them if they need someone to do it with first.

6. Make your action plan your friend

Oh, they can be a pain to write at first, but wow can they be useful for keeping you on track, not trying to save the world too much and also for being able to reflect on how great you actually are at your job. A tight plan enables you to do more of what matters, broken down into specific objectives and with clear success criteria and milestones. Make sure your planned actions are high value: how will you enable improvements in the focus area? We recommend the ‘actions’ section is primarily about CPD, focused on the subject knowledge or pedagogical gaps that need to be addressed and using a range of strategies for how you will address them. Tip: if it’s all about the staff meetings, think outside the box. In what other ways can you make a difference (and often a bigger difference)?

One of the threads running through this year’s Subject Leader training was about coaching a colleague in their school. They looked at children’s work, assessment information and used their other monitoring outcomes to identify who to work with and on which areas. Then, through a combination of planning support and often team-teaching too, they have helped that colleague improve their subject knowledge or pedagogy, or both.  In this way, SLs were able to really pinpoint the key changes they were seeing, celebrate their impact and to refine their own coaching skills as well.

7. Stop counting books!

No, I mean it. Stop. If you are the one counting how many books in the Guided Reading sets per year group then your talents are going to waste. Can you delegate? Are there some volunteers, or even keen Y6 children, who can do these sorts of tasks for you, once shown how? Yes, you need to keep an eye on resources and replenishment/gap-filling, but really and honestly the biggest (and most expensive) resource in a school is the human one: people’s brains. You need to reserve your precious time for working with people, helping them reflect, evaluate and move themselves forwards. My course co-trainer, Theresa Clements, reminded me of a brilliant quote:

“Leaders don’t create followers, they create more leaders.” (Tom Peters)

This really spoke to our subject leaders on the course, as all the way along they have been thinking ‘How do I enable this colleague to carry on doing X independently, after my support?’. That’s what you’re after really isn’t it? Show them your planning (for example), have a go together, now let them go it alone. It’s the same scaffolding model as when we work with little learners too: I can, we can, you can. When embarking on a piece of support always think about how you will enable them to carry on on their own; have an exit strategy.

8. Be positive

It’s not all doom and gloom. No matter how much still needs to be done, there are always green shoots of your impact if you look for them. Maybe keep a little note-book of things you have seen, times when that chat over the coffee urn has paid off and ‘thank you’s from parents or colleagues etc. It’s motivating for your colleagues – and for you! – to keep remembering all the good things going on, and it really helps pave the way for other favours you may need to ask, so remember to say thank you or well done to others yourself too.

9. Build capacity

Just as attending the BHESL course was an investment in capacity-building by the headteachers, fully participating in the course is an investment for the SLs too; investment of time, trust and commitment. It works because we weave the whole course through the school improvement cycle, helping SLs to carry out the very tasks they would be needing to do for themselves anyway but in a collaborative and safe atmosphere where they are supported to go for the hardest parts of the job and constantly encouraged to keep challenging themselves. It’s a unique course. It’s a course people always remember. I recall attending its Grandpappy ‘SLIPs’ (Subject Leadership in Primary Schools…anyone else remember that from c.2005?!). It’s the kind of course where it changes you as a leader and sets you on the right path to doing the things that matter, getting into the right habits, and developing your own skills as a leader. Who knows where it could take you in the future! I often speak to deputies and heads who fondly remember coming on SLIPs in the past (“It totally changed me” said one). Well, it has evolved, quite rightly, into something very robust, very reflective and very much geared towards the higher expectations of NC2014 and Ofsted schedules of today.

10. Plan ahead

Start thinking now about ‘what about after I’m gone?’. The real proof of the pudding about any of your initiatives, projects, or policies will be whether the impact continues after you move on. Will people forget how to do x? Will the edges be rubbed off y? Are there systems built into the school year to ensure things are not forgotten and don’t fall off the radar, or do these improvements you’ve worked so hard to achieve depend on you being there? Again, thinking about how highly effective leaders “create more leaders”, have you instilled in your colleagues some new ways of thinking, so that they have not just the enhanced subject knowledge but also the skills and reflectiveness to sustain those continual improvements and to keep the school journeying ever-upwards..? Quite often on a school staff there is another colleague who is almost as passionate about your subject as you are. Perhaps they might like to shadow you doing some of your SL role, and maybe – just maybe – they could be the one to carry the torch when you are gone.

Share this

Steps to spelling: track back to leap forward

Published
27 March 2018

man leaping

 

 What’s the issue?

In 2014, the National Curriculum catapulted spelling at KS2 into the limelight. The 2016 KS2 interim assessment framework document further intensified the focus on spelling, as children could not be judged as ‘working at the expected standard’ if they had not first demonstrated that they could spell most words correctly from the years 3/4, and then from the years 5/6 programme of study. The ‘spelling geek’ inside me was quietly thrilled as were many of the teachers and colleagues I work with. Not because of the prescriptive way it had been ‘forced’ upon us by a strict secure fit model (ITAF), more that finally, spelling wasn’t having to play second fiddle to other aspects of writing. There was an overwhelming feeling that something more had to be done about spelling, and not just at Year 6, but throughout KS2.

My colleague, Penny Slater, had previously stated in a blog that the side-lining of spelling had ‘led to a skewed perception of spelling as an add-on to the process of becoming literate, rather than an integral part of it’. This statement really rang true with me, and seemed to be a message that I too had been led to believe during the early stages of my teaching career.

I know not everyone celebrated this ‘catapulting’ in the same way I did, and I completely understand reservations, or preferences towards a more creative and flexible approach to writing and composition that takes into consideration a child’s particular strengths and weaknesses. However, in my experience, the more fluent a child becomes in spelling (and handwriting) the more they are cognitively freed up to focus on the composition, so to my mind it’s a win either way.

A whole school approach

It was during this period I started working on a project with the English Subject Leader and staff at Oakmere Primary School, in Potters Bar, Hertfordshire. The number of children achieving the EXS standard had increased in writing by 24% from 2016 to 2017 to 70% However, monitoring including book scrutinies, and discussions with teachers still identified the teaching and application of spelling in writing as a key development priority across KS2. The school and Subject Leader were keen to raise standards and try something new. We agreed to focus specifically on plugging gaps for pupils that were struggling, and focus on how to manage teaching ARE, whilst still meeting the needs of pupils who were struggling with spelling.

I delivered an INSET at the beginning of this academic year. During the INSET, the teachers buddied up and had a detailed look at the spelling of a group of low-attaining pupils in their class. They analysed the spelling errors for those pupils and then used this to identify key areas to prioritise in teaching. A spelling track back document was then used to see which year group misconceptions had stemmed from; consideration was given to how to build on the ‘last good’ bit of learning and how to link their pupils’ gaps to their year group expectations whilst teaching the whole class.

Once the gaps for the children had been identified, teachers began grappling with a slightly different way of planning and delivering the teaching of spelling.

See below an example of the Steps to Spelling document which can used to:

  • track back and identify where any misconceptions or gaps in learning may be present;
  • differentiate for pupils who need to accelerate to age-related expectations;
  • support teachers in securing their subject knowledge.

spelling ladder

The Steps to Spelling and Essential Spelling resources are now available from the HFL Education shop.

Just a couple of days after the INSET, one of the teachers tweeted the following reflection:

spelling tweet

 

I could not have summed it up better myself. In his reflections, I think his tweet really hit the nail on the head. In the past, I too, had used these methods (including hope), and oddly for some of my pupils this had actually worked. However, to really make a significant difference to children’s learning we have to have deep knowledge of the areas we are teaching: ‘hope’ is no longer good enough.

The Sutton Trust review acknowledges the importance of teachers having very secure, deep subject knowledge if they want to support children in tackling tricky concepts:

When teachers’ knowledge falls below a certain level it is a significant impediment to students’ learning. As well as a strong understanding of the material being taught, teachers must also understand the ways students think about the content, be able to evaluate the thinking behind students’ own methods, and identify students’ common misconceptions. (Coe R, Aloisi C, Higgins S and Major L.E. What makes great teaching? Review of the underpinning research. October 2014)

To summarise the approach taken by the case-study school:

  • develop teachers’ subject knowledge in order for them to identify misconceptions accurately;
  • complete a gap analysis of pupils’ spelling errors in their independent writing;
  • prioritise key learning areas to focus on from the analysis;
  • use the analysis to track back to where the pupils are working from;
  • use the track back information to teach from and build on the last ‘good’ bit of learning.

Five months on at Oakmere, many parents from the Year 2 class have reported that they have noticed an improvement in their child's spelling ability and confidence to spell, and the class teacher has commented that it appears to be having a good impact on the pupils’ writing. In Years 4 and 5, they have now established a really effective sequence of assessing: teaching in differentiated groups based on the tracking back that feeds in to the initial sound/spelling statement and a final assessment to track progress. In Year 6, some children have made progress, but there still seems to be a group who have been frustratingly resistant to making progress. The class teacher feels this may be due to individual factors that influence the effectiveness of their learning. I will be considering these pupils and possible next steps in the second part of this blog.

I can’t pretend that for many children the way we have always taught spelling and still do (for some) hasn’t worked, because it has. It just doesn’t work for all of the children all of the time, and if what you’re doing isn’t working, then you’re just going to have to try something else. If you are looking for that ‘something else’, then the approach used at Oakmere School might be worth a try.

With thanks to Oakmere School, Potters Bar, Herts.

Blog written by Sabrina Wright.

Share this

All hail Macbeth!

Published
23 March 2018

It fills me with such delight that more and more schools are reading Shakespeare with children in the primary phase. There are many wonderful theatre groups now working with schools on this too. It can be really tricky to get that first encounter right. If we select a play too complicated, too dark or too romantic, we can inadvertently put children off future exploration. There are a few great contenders for KS2 in my opinion. A Midsummer Night’s Dream offers the ‘play within a play’; I have seen children delighting over the antics of Bottom and his motley crew in the classroom. I distinctly recall being utterly swept up with the magic of the forest and the fairies as a child of 10 or 11 – this was my first encounter with Shakespeare and I’ve been hooked ever since. My personal favourite for teaching in upper key stage two though, is Macbeth.

This is Shakespeare’s shortest play so it is possible to explore the entire play over a teaching sequence. The scenes with the three witches are deliciously fun for children to (over)act and we also have murder, betrayal, ghosts and battle to plunder and explore. I have won the hearts and minds over of many a reluctant reader through walking in role across the heath, cackling in role as a witch or descending into madness in role as Macbeth. There ought to be a finely-struck balance between reading - and acting - the original Shakespearean text, and reading extracts from narrative versions to a) get through the entire play and b) assimilate the complicated structure of the plot. When I say acting here, I don’t necessarily mean adorning robes and taking to the stage. Many schools do decide to link a Shakespeare unit of work with a school performance and this is a wonderful and memorable opportunity for children. But for some pupils, this would fill them with dread. I advocate meaning-laden read alouds in class from the text, with plenty of time for peer collaboration and rehearsal. Reader’s Theatre is a brilliant strategy for facilitating repeat re-reading in an inclusive and non-threatening way, whilst allowing plenty of freedom for those keen and able to express themselves dramatically. Other drama pedagogical approaches such as Paired Improvisation and Freeze-framing also work brilliantly – all this can be done in the classroom, in the English lesson.

Not only will a rich and exciting unit on Macbeth satisfy the statutory requirement that children read from a wide range of books, including fiction from our literary heritage, it can also inspire some superb writing outcomes too. Pupils in year 5 at St. John’s Catholic Primary School in Rickmansworth, Hertfordshire, used HfL’s Detailed Exemplification Planning to explore Macbeth. An overview of the 5-week long plan is available as a free download here. During this lesson towards the start of the unit, pupils enjoyed exploring the heath, acting in role:

Macbeth%20planning.jpg

This is an example of writing generated at the end of the lesson:

Macbeth%20writing.jpg

The opportunity afforded to pupils here to enter the world of the text and to experience being on the heath first-hand has greatly enhanced the writing. The reader is drawn in and carefully led through this dramatic setting through controlled use of noun phrases and precise vocabulary choices – all acquired by this pupil during the drama. How delighted I would be by writing of this quality at the start of a unit!

Pupils are asked, as another incidental writing task early on in the unit, to write a letter in role as Lady Macbeth, demonstrating their understanding of the prophecies and their recognition of Lady Macbeth’s murderous intent...

diary

 

 

diary

 

Not only have the children at St John’s absolutely loved reading and acting from Macbeth in class, they have also been producing writing rich with audience and purpose that they are keen to share.

Macbeth%20display.jpg

 

 

book

 

You can access the Macbeth Detailed Exemplification Plan in full here, along with detailed plans for other year groups in key stage one and two. A free download of the overview is available on the TES website here.

Huge thanks to Nicola O’Brien and year 5 pupils at St John’s Catholic Primary School in Rickmansworth, Hertfordshire.

Share this